I have previously written about how, in the 16th
century Franciscan friars believed that St. Thomas the apostle had visited
Mexico and preached Christianity to the natives 1500 years before the arrival
of the Spaniards, and that the Indians themselves were descendants of one of
the lost tribes of Israel. The Franciscans saw similarities between Indigenous
religion and Christianity that they explained to themselves in this way. Today
such a belief of visits from the Ancient Near East to Mesoamerica is not common
among Christian denominations, but it is found today among the Latter Day
Saints (also known as Mormons), whose sacred book, the Book of Mormon tells that four Hebrew
tribes made it to the Americas. Where their prophets wrote the original Book of Mormon on metal plates in a language named "Reformed Egyptian". LDS scholars have, over the years, invested
much time and energy in trying to find external evidence in support of the account given
in the Book of Mormon, both through archaeology and linguistics.
Front page of the work under review. |
Usually, I would follow Jay S. Gould in considering
scientific inquiry and religious confession to be non-overlapping magisteria, and
that as long as scholars keep their religious beliefs out of their scientific
inquiries then they can believe whatever they want. But sometimes this is not
so easy, and this blogpost is about one of those times.
In this blogpost, I analyze the use of Nahuatl data, in
Brian D. Stubbs self-published manuscript “Exploring the Explanatory Power of Semitic and Egyptian in Uto-Aztecan”. This work compares Proto-Uto-Aztecan
with Semitic and Egyptian and seeks to find signs of ancient contact between
early near-eastern and Egyptian peoples and Uto-Aztecan speaking Native
Americans. It finds a lot of such signs, in fact more than 1500 Uto-Aztecan forms
that Stubbs claims are cognate with Semitic or Egyptian forms.
Now, far-fetched proposals of relations between languages
that are never known to have been in contact and which defy the conventional
view of world history are a dime a
dozen. I have already described Denison’s attempt to show that Nahuatl was an“Aryan language”, and Turkic nationalists frequently look to Nahuatl when
seeking to explain their belief that all languages descend from the Turkic “Sun Language”. Usually, I would say that it is better not to give too much
attention to far fetched claims of long distance contact across the Atlantic,
and better not to waste one’s energy trying to debunk them since they are
usually not playing a game in which the scientific rules and methods of
historical linguistics even apply. But one thing makes this proposal different:
Namely the fact that Brian D. Stubbs is a well-esteemed expert in Uto-Aztecan
historical linguistics with many important publications to his name, not least
his monumental catalogue of Uto-Aztecan cognate sets (Stubbs 2011). This he is, in addition to
being also, apparently, a member of the LDS church. This makes for an
interesting conundrum, because it both gives us an apriori reason to believe in the
validity of his claims because of his expertise, and an apriori reason for doubting it,
because of the fact that his claim is clearly meant to validate the narrative
of the Book of Mormon, and his own religious belief. (it is certainly taken as such validation by other LDS members and publicized as such)
I have a great deal of
respect and esteem for Stubbs’ previous Uto-Aztecan comparative work, but the claim of ancient
trans-Atlantic contact, even disregarding any potential religious motivation behind it,
is a sufficiently extraordinary claim, for it to require inordinately
extraordinary evidence in its favor, before it can be accepted.
C.S. Lewis coined the term ‘bulverism’ to describe the kind
of argument in which one simply assumes that one’s opponent in a discussion is
wrong, and then proceeds to explain to him the psychological mechanism that
must have led them astray. If we want to avoid making ourselves guilty of
bulverism, we cannot simply say “oh this is just religious crack-pottery” and
leave it at that. We have to actually show, that Stubbs’ work in this case does
not follow what is expected from rigorous linguistic scholarship, and that its
conclusions therefore cannot be accepted as valid.
The manuscript is 444 pages and includes some, 1500 proposed cognates, so it would require quite a lot of effort to analyze all of it, so
instead I will limit my analysis here to Stubbs' treatment of Nahuatl. Nahuatl being of
course my field of specialization, and the area where I will be most likely to
see weak spots in his argument and catch any methodological blunders.
Previous Reviews: Roberts, Elzinga and Rogers
I am not the
first to analyze or review Stubbs' Semitic/Uto-Aztecan work. It has been previously reviewed by three Brigham Young University linguists all specializing in Native American languages: Dirk Elzinga, John S. Robertson and Chris Rogers. It does appear, though, that my review here is the first written by a linguist who is not affiliated with the LDS-owned BYU, and not published in a LDS related journal.
Published in the "Interpreter: A Journal of Latter-day Saints faith and scholarship", the review by John Robertson, a specialist in the Maya language, is enthusiastic and overall accepting. Robertson concludes that he "cannot find an easy way to challenge the breadth and depth of the data".
Published in "BYU Studies Quarterly", the review by Dirk Elzinga (a Uto-Aztecanist) is what I would call lukewarm, concluding that though the proposal looks like normal crackpottery at first glance, the authority and expertise of the author means that it merits further attention.
Published in the "Journal of Book of Mormon Studies" in 2019, the review by Chris Rogers,
an expert in historical linguistics and the Xinka language of El Salvador and
also a professor of linguistics at Brigham Young University, is much more critical.
Rogers points out some
serious flaws in the work: Primary of these is that it does not stick to the
established bilateral method of comparing languages with languages and
proto-languages with proto-languages, but that it frequently cherrypicks so that
a form in any Semitic or Egyptian variety can be compared with a form in any
Uto-Aztecan language. As has been demonstrated time and time again this
multilateral method hugely increases the risk of mistaking chance resemblances for cognates, and makes
it possible to prove virtually anything. As Rogers’ points out since any two
languages can be expected to have between 1% and 3% chance cognates, if we add
additional languages to the comparison the risk rises incrementally as well.
This means that once Stubbs is comparing 30 Uto-Aztecan languages with at least
three Semitic and Egyptian varieties (actually more, including at least Egyptian, Coptic, Syriac,
Hebrew, Aramaic) the number of expected chance similarities far exceeds the
1528 proposed cognates. He also notes that Stubbs does not adequately explain
non-matching segments (in fact no explanation at all is given in most cases),
and that he does not adequately account for other potential explanations of the
similarity (such as onomatopoeia). These general criticisms seem absolutely
valid to me, now on having reviewed Stubbs' work myself.
In a response to Rogers’ review published as a pdf on the academia.edu site, Stubbs defends himself by
stating first of all that Rogers has misunderstood his intent: He is not trying
to prove that Uto-Aztecan and Semitic/Egyptian are genetically related, but
that his claim is rather that proto-Uto-Aztecan was a mixed language that acquired a
major portion of its vocabulary and grammar from speakers of these Afroasiatic languages.
Therefore, it seems to follow, he does not need to follow the strict methods for
demonstrating long distance relationships. As for the charge of cherry picking, he
claims that it is only natural that some forms borrowed into the proto-langage
survive only in some of the daughter languages. This is perhaps true, but he apparently
does not recognize, or address, the fact that this practice leads
to a much higher risk of chance resemblance being mistaken for cognates, that is, random noise being mistaken for a signal. Furthermore, it cannot simply be assumed that a given form in a daughter language is a retention just because it looks like something in Hebrew, it has to be demonstrated that it is not an innovation in the individual language.
Though I can see why Rogers thought that Stubbs was arguing for a genetic relation given Stubbs presentation of the evidence, I wish Rogers had realized that Stubbs’ claim was in fact a proposal of
language contact. Because it really is a more problematic claim.
It is
problematic because there is no accepted method for demonstrating borrowing or
contact induced changes, and consequently no method for falsifying them.
Systematic sound laws do not apply in the transfer of elements of one language
to another, a language may borrow many words or few and change them in fairly random ways as they are adapted to the borrowing language's phonology, and there really is no
good way to disprove a claim about a form in one prehistoric language being
borrowed from another. This is why linguists normally would never even entertain the idea of a scenario of borrowing, unless there is independent evidence suggesting
probable historical contact between the two languages. In this, case there is
exactly zero independent evidence of contact between Ancient Semites or Egyptians and
Uto-Aztecans…except for the Book of Mormon.
So by presenting his hypothesis as
a claim of ancient contact and language mixture, Stubbs is in fact making a
claim that cannot be methodically falsified. When working outside of historical
disciplines, such a claim is usually called pseudoscientific, but in a historical
discipline such as this, we can only strive to classify it either as convincing
or unconvincing given the presented evidence.
Analysis
So, to the evidence: In the following, I will assess as many
examples as I could find of Stubbs using Nahuatl data to support a claimed cognate set. I am not looking at those parts where he uses Nahuatl as one language among many to support a PUA reconstruction, but only those where Nahuatl is the only support for a claimed relation across the families. Grantedly, most of his comparisons
are to his own reconstructed proto-Uto-Aztecan forms, but he does on many
occasions reach into Nahuan and compare Nahuan forms directly with forms in
individual semitic varieties. Looking closely at these examples will give us feel for how
Stubbs handles linguistic data - and specifically whether Stubbs' claim that dipping in to the individual languages is justified because these forms are retentions, is itself justified.
Shadow:
šwt ‘shade,
shadow’ > Nahuatl seewal-li
‘shade’
This example illustrates several problems. First is the fact
that semitic and Egyptian roots are triconsonantal, whereas Uto-Aztecan roots
almost always are CV or CVC roots. This means that from the outset when
comparing a form across the languages you have to ignore all the vowels and
look only at the consonants. This of course raises the number of potential
cognates on the Uto-Aztecan side quite a lot since you can ignore basically
half of every word. Secondly, Stubbs argues that all Semitic sibilants (three
different ones) are reflected as *s in Uto-Aztecan. So again, this kind of
merger, raises the number of potential cognates on the Semitic side. So now we have quite a large pool of potentially cognate
words in each of the language families, the only thing needed for a chance
resemblance to appear is if two of them have the same meaning. The greater the number of potential cognates, the greater also the chance for a semantic match or near match. Usually when
demonstrating a genetic relation only words with the exact same meaning are accepted,
since if we now also relax the degree of semantic fit we require, then the risk
of chance resemblances increase even more. Here, the fit seems to be quite
good, superficially. Because though Classical Nahuatl seewalli refers to 'shade', it does not do so etymologically. The etymology points to an original meaning of "a cool place", or "something that has cooled down". We know this, because it is composed of four different morphemes. The morpheme see ‘cold’ and
the verbal formant –wa form the verb seewa ‘to be cold’, the –l suffix is an
old passive form, and the –li ending is the absolutive suffix. So the word in
Nahuatl is analyzable into mono-consonantal roots each with a distinct meaning
that comes together to become ‘cold place’ which is then extended to also mean
‘shade’, and the analysis is not at all compatible with the proposed Semitic
cognate which is a triconsonantal root.
Bow/Rainbow
šmrt ‘large
bow’, pl šmrwt > -samaaloo-t of Nahuatl koo-samaaloo-tl ‘rainbow’
This example also shows the problems mentioned above, of
neglecting to analyze the Nahuatl word into its roots. What is basically being
compared here is šmrt with smlt, this can only be done because Stubbs decides
that Nahuatl ko- is a prefix that can be removed, and because he neglects to
remove the final –t which *is* a suffix and *should* be removed. But ko is not
likely to be a prefix, indeed the likely historical analysis of the Nahuatl
word is kosa-ma-l-o-tl, where the kosa- root is found in the word for yellow
kos-tik and the word for becoming yellow kosawi, and the word for necklace
koskatl. So he should compare šmrt with ksml, but that doesn’t look very much a
like at all. Then there is the problem of semantic stretch, he is comparing two
words that are related in English “bow” and “rainbow”, but why assume that this
is a natural semantic connection? Nahuatl has other words for the weapon, and
no word for the architectural feature, the Nahuatl word does not appear related to the shape at all, but
rather to the color, and Nahua myth compares it to a snake not a bow.
Reed
twr ‘reed’ > Nahuatl tool-in ‘cattails, reeds’;
This actually seems reasonable to me since I would probably
reconstruct Nahuatl toolin as coming from an earlier form along the lines of *tawri. But still we are only matching three segments out of five, which means
the risk of chance resemblance is high.
Son/Child
Hebrew bεn ‘son’; pl: bəneey3‘children (of)’ >
Nahuatl *konee 'child, offspring’:
This one is a far stretch phonologically with only the –n
being a direct match between the two forms. Stubbs argues that PUA *kw may be
correspond to Semitic *b, but konee doesn't come from a proto-form with kw (As Stubbs notes Nahuatl is one of the only UA languages to keep PUA *kw as kw). In fact I think it probably comes from the PUA root *kumCa reconstructed by Stubbs (2011) as meaning "husband" and as "male".
Serpent/Twin
Egyptian qrђt ‘serpent’, Egyptian qrђ ‘friend,
partner’ > UA/Nahuatl koŋwa ‘snake, twin’
Here it is certainly interesting that both Egyptian and
Nahuatl have a word that means both snake and a human friend/partner/twin. Within Uto-Aztecan, Nahuatl is to my knowledge the only language that has this double
meaning of the word for snake, and thus the double meaning cannot be reconstructed for PUA.
And the
phonological form is quite far from each other: really there isn’t even a
single segment that can be considered a direct match between the two languages.
One has to accept Stubbs’ complicated sound correspondences where multiple
Semitic/Egyptian segments match a single segment in Nahuatl and in which vowels
are irrelevant, and consonant segments can dissappear. In this particular
instance, he has to insert an n to get an nw cluster that can correspond to rђ,
and to get this n into the word he cites a 1976 paper by Joe Campbell with
which I am very familiar (it is about Hueyapan Nahuatl, which is my main
expertise among Nahuatl varieties). First of all, Campbell does not in fact
argue that there is a historical /ŋw/ in any variety of Nahuatl, he only makes
the argument that synchronically there can be posited an underlying
hypothetical *ŋw phoneme that explains some irregularities in the grammar. Joe
has confirmed to me several times that he did not mean to make a historical
argument, but was only making a synchronic phonological argument in the style
of the structuralism of the 1970s. Secondly, Joe does say that the hypothetical
ŋw element is found in the word ko:wa,
but he is talking about the verb ko:wa
“to buy”, not the noun ko:watl “snake”. So it is a completely different unrelated
word. The reference to Joe’s article to justify the claimed medial /ŋw/ is both
a case of special pleading, and of misusing another scholar’s work in doing so.
Crocodile
The Egyptian Crocodile God Sobek (photo Hedwig Storck, WikiCommons) |
Egyptian sbk ‘crocodile, the crocodile-god Sobek’ and
Classical Nahuatl sipak-tli ‘crocodile’.
Here again we have a good superficial likeness with actual
near match of all three consonants – and Stubbs’ further argues that in
Egyptian the voweling matches the Nahuatl form as well. We need to abstract, of course, from the fact that elsewhere the comparisons is between Nahuatl and Hebrew, Nahuatl and Maghrib Arabic, Nahuatl and Aramaic, and here Nahuatl and Egyptian (which is not even a Semitic language but related in the much larger Afro-Asiatic family).
Aztec crocodile god Cipactli, from the Codex Borgia |
The problem is
that we cannot reconstruct this word for crocodile for proto-Uto-Aztecan, because
no other Uto-Aztecan language has a documented cognate of the word sipaktli. There is a possibility that the word could be related to the Corachol word for caiman háaxi, where the first syllable likely means "water". The second syllable -xi could then be cognate to the si- syllable of sipaktli (and in fact the pa- syllable could be cognate to the ha- syllable of corachol, the order of the elements in the compound simply reversed). This would make sipaktli a likely compound word, in which case it cannot match the Egyptian triconsonantal root at all.
Wine-skin/Prickly
pear
"Hebrew nebεl ‘skin-bottle, skin’ in the common phrase
of Hebrew nebεl yayin ‘skin of wine’; Syriac nbl3/3n’bl > Classical Nahuatl
no’palli ‘prickly pear’ often used to make alcoholic beverage";
Here we have an
ok phonological match (though it unexplainedly ignores the Nahuatl Saltillo segment),
but a very bad semantic match.
The Nahuatl word nohpalli quite simply does not mean prickly
pear, it refers to the opuntia cactus, the prickly pear of which is called
noochtli. Nohpalitl, also refers specifically to the edible ear of the cactus. Though mostly eaten as a fruit, the noochtli was used to produce a kind of fermented beverage called noochoctli. But the nohpal cactus is only tied to alcohol production in this very indirect way. Allowing this span from wineskin to cactus with a fruit used occasionally for fermented shows an very high degree of semantic latitude, and a cavalier approach to translation since nohpal is *not* the prickly pear or the part of the plant used for fermentation.
Bury/Tamal
Semitic ṭmn > Aramaic ṭmr ‘hide, bury’ > Nahuatl
tamal-li ‘tamale’
Stubbs argues that the Semitic root *tmn had the “references
to ‘cooking underground or under ashes’ …which in Post-Biblical Hebrew also
meant ‘put in an oven’” He makes the final l- in the Nahuatl fit by noting that “Aramaic
changed n > r, as it often does (ben ‘son’ > bar ‘son’)”.
But again, he
doesn’t take the time to analyze the Nahuatl word, which does indeed come from
a verb that can be reconstructed as *tɨma with the meaning “cook with
steam/bake under ground”. But the final –l in tamal, is not a part of the root but a suffix, it is as mentioned before an
old passive that is used to derive deverbal passive nouns, so a tamal is
analyzable in Nahuatl and means “something steamed”. So again we have only two consonants out of three (or if counting the vowels two segments out of five) that actually match.
Perfective prefix
Semitic perfective with wa- Nahuatl perfective with o-.
Stubbs notes that in some Semitic verbs a perfective can be
formed by adding the prefix wa- and removing the last vowel of the verb. This
is indeed quite similar to what happens in Western Nahuatl dialects, where an
o- prefix and the loss of the last vowel creates the perfective of one verb
class. But, this is limited to Western Nahuatl dialects, the Eastern dialects
have neither the o- prefix nor the dropping of the stemfinal vowel in any verb
class (they use a –k or –ki suffix instead). The vowel-dropping in preterit
forms cannot be reconstructed for proto-Nahuatl, but is an innovation in the
Western Branch. Karen Dakin has argued that Western Nahuatl ot the o- prefix as
a borrowing from the corachol perfective prefix wa-. And it probably also
shouldn’t be reconstructed for proto-Nahuatl. So here we have a pattern that is
superficially intriguingly similar, but once we know a bit about Nahuatl
historical developments it disappears entirely.
Climb up/on top
Semitic rkb ‘mount, climb up on’ > CN tlakpa-k
‘above, on top’
Again Stubbs compares a triconsonantal root to a
multimorphemic Nahuat word. The tla- in tlakpak is a prefix, that was
originally ta-, and the root ikpa comes from the PUA root *kupa ‘hair’ or ‘head’
and has come to mean “top”. The root rkb
‘mount climb up’ is not a very good match for kupa ‘hair/head’.
Quail
Hebrew śәlaaw
‘quail’, pl:salwiim; Syriac salway ‘quail’; Arabic salwaa ‘quail’; Samaritan
šalwi > UA *solwi ‘quail’: CN sool-in ‘quail’; Mn sowi’ ‘pigeon’.
Here we have something that is superficially interesting
again. But we have to note that ' reconstruction of the UA term combines the Mono word for "pigeon" with the Nahuatl word for quail to get an almost exact match for the Semitic word. But he has missed the obvious cognates in Corachol, namely Huichol xïau “codorniz” and Cora sa’uh. These forms, I reconstruct for proto-Corachol as *sauri, which is also the ancestral form for the Nahuatl word sool-in. Then we end up comparing
slw with swr (if we admit the correspondence u/w), and we would have to posit a metathesis to sustain the argument.
Understand/Grow
Hebrew
hiśkiil, hiskal- ‘to understand, comprehend, make wise’ > CN iskal ‘to
train’; CN iskal-ia ‘be discreet, prudent’.
Simeon's entry for Izcalia |
Here we have something really bad. Namely an example of a massaged translation of the Nahuatl term that
makes a large semantic stretch seem less problematic. Nahuatl iskalia does not mean "be discreet prudent". The intransitive iskali means “to grow” or "to revive" for example about plants that sprout. From this is derived the transitive iskalia that means "to make someone revive or grow" and which is used metaphorically in the sense of coming to one's senses for example after having passed out (used reflexively "make oneself revive"), and in the sense of nurturing and rearing a child to maturity (like one cultivates a plant). In Hueyapan one does respond to someone who says something silly by saying "ximoskali!", which is literally "come to your senses!". But is is related to reviving and coming back to life, and not to understanding or knowing.
Molina's entry for Izcalia with no "prudence or discretion" |
Stubbs' seems to have the "discreet, prudent" translation from the dictionary of Remi Simeon where "discreto, prudente" appears in a string of words used to translate iskalia (image inserted right). Simeon's dictionary is a derivative, mostly based on Alonso de Molina's dictionary, and interestingly the "discreet, prudent" doesn't appear as possible translations of the words in Molina's dictionary (image inserted). They were added by Simeon. The gloss "to train" for the putative word "iskal" is simply made up perhaps based on the metaphorical meaning of iskalia "to rear" a child. "Iskal" meanwhile, is not an actually existing Nahuatl word, since as I am sure Stubbs knows, all Nahuatl verbs end in a vowel.
But of course when comparing, one has to try to analyze the word's core semantics instead of the simply choosing one of the potential translations that you like best. Here the semantics simply doesn't fit, and instead of analyzing the word's meaning Stubbs simply cherry picks the two of Simeon's glosses that fit best with his Hebrew word.
Select/Take
Hebrew(BDB) brr ‘to select, choose’: CN kwia / kwiya
‘to consider s.th. one’s own, to keep’; CN kwi-lia ‘to take s.th.’;
This is also an example of a massaged translation, because Nahuatl
kwi means simply to take something, kwia is a derived verb that means "to wrap something" and kwilia is the applicative of kwi that means "to
take something from someone". Simeon again has a meaning of kwia not found in other dictionaries, namely the meaning of "keeping something borrowed". This could be a potential contextual extension of the sense of "taking something", with the added -a, to signify that it is used transitively. But given that there is no other source for this usage than Simeon's dictionary, that is not very accurate, and often supplies extra translations based on unknown evidence, it seems a bad idea to pick this specific meaning to compare with Hebrew.
Apart from this semantic mismatch, the only
element that actually matches is the b/kw. The r segment matches neither the l
in kwilia because this is the applicative suffix. The y in kwiya suggested by Simeon is spuriously inserted because
if there was the preterit would be *kwix, but is in fact kwih. Hence the *y cannot be used to match the r in the semitic form.
Pronominals
One of the really eye-catching pieces of data presented by Stubbs is the correspondence between the semitic pronominal prefixes and the pronouns of Classical Nahuatl. He notes that the Aramaic verb 'to be' parallels the Nahuatl pronouns closely, being 'ehwe "I am", tehwe "you are" and yehwe "he is". And he notes that Maghrib Arabic analogized the first person plural n- to become first person singular as in Nahuatl. Here, I reproduce Stubbs' table from page 335.
This kind of paradigm is the kind that historical linguists love, because it is a kind of relation that is relatively unlikely to arise by coincidence. But though it may be unlikely to arise by chance, it is not impossible. In this case in fact very possible.
In their reviews, both Elzinga and Robertson catch on this example as the most convincing piece of evidence. Rogers though mentions the table critically, by pointing out the problem that Stubbs is here comparing individual varieties, and not proto-languages. Here Rogers points out a problem that in fact invalidates this example: Again the Nahuatl forms represent innovations within Nahuatl not retentions of earlier PUA forms, and hence cannot be used as examples of contact allegedly taking place thousands of years before proto-Nahuatl emerged.
The reason Stubbs' compares with Classical Nahuatl instead of PUA, or even instead of proto-Nahuatl is clear: this particular pronoun system is *only* found in Nahuatl in the entire UA family, and consequently cannot be reconstructed for the earlier stage. In fact, we can show that it is an innovation in proto-Nahuatl by comparison with the forms found in Corachol. In proto-Corachol the paradigm was 1p ne-, 2p pa- 3p pu-. Stubbs himself in his catalogue of UA cognates reconstructs pu- as the third person singular pronominal in PUA. From a system similar to that found in corachol, Nahuatl switched the second person form, apparently adopting the ti- prefix of the first person plural, also as the second person singular prefix. It kept the ni- prefix and it dropped the third person prefix altogether since it was redundant. The pu- pronominal stem was in fact only kept in the third person pronouns which in proto-Nahuatl I reconstruct as *yeha from an earlier *puha. The initial PUA syllable *pu becomes *hɨ- in proto-Corachol-Nahuatl, then ye- in proto-Nahuatl and then e- in eastern Nahuatl (except in the pronoun because Eastern Nahuatl had changed the pronoun to yaha, and y- was only dropped before e). The original pronoun furthermore did not have the -wa suffix, which is an innovation in the Western branch of Nahuan (so quite late, after the split of proto-Nahuatl). Consequently, the forms of the Aramaic copula and Mahgrib pronominal prefixes are complete red herrings, they only compare to the pronouns of Western Nahuatl in the postclassic period (i.e. after 900 AD).
Moreover, the Maghrib first person form n- is an innovation while in Uto-Aztecan it is a retention, and it is probably even a retention from a much earlier stage of language evolution in the Americas given that many other language families in the Americas have ni- as the first person singular pronominal. So really if we were to allege contact, it would suggest that Maghrib Arabic was influenced by Nahuatl (or another Native American language), rather than the other way round. But of course this is really just a coincidence, and not evidence of contact at all.
If we were to compare only the oldest reconstructible stages of the two languages we would get:
Semitic Proto-Corachol-Nahua
1.sg '- ne-
2p.sg. t- pe-
3p.sg. y- pu-/Ø-
1p.pl. n- t-
2p.pl t- se-
3p.pl y- me-
Here, nothing at all is shared between the two systems. (I presented my preliminary reconstruction of elements of proto-Corachol-Nahua at the Friends of Uto-Aztecan meetings in Tepic in 2018 including the pronominal prefixes, the proceedings are
Starting from two systems that didn't actually share anything, Proto-Nahuatl swapped a previous 2p.sg prefix pa- to ti-, and the changed the form of the 3p.sg. prefix pu- to ye-, and Mahgrib swapped 1p '- to n-, and voilá: the systems align.
In conclusion, the pronoun system that Stubbs compares with semitic is much too late for being evidence of a relation between PUA and Semitic, since it came into existence only several thousand years after the existence of PUA and the alleged contact between Semites, Egyptians and Uto-Aztecans. This is a case in which we can in fact show that similar pronominal paradigms have developed independently of each other.
Conclusions: It’s a no from me
For all these reasons, I find the proposal to be very far
from convincing. It seems to me that here, Stubbs is not at all doing the kind
of careful comparative work that he is known for. The handling of Nahuatl data
is highly problematic, with massaged translations that make words that have
virtually no semantic link falsely appear to have the same meanings, with apparently selective failure
to pay attention to segmentation and morphological analysis in the Nahuatl, and
inattention to other possible and plausible explanations even those found in his own previous work.
I should note, that since many of the reconstructions of proto-Corachol and proto-Corachol Nahuatl are my own and most of them yet unpublished, Stubbs cannot of course be faulted for not knowing or accepting them. But if anyone in the world would be equipped with enough knowledge to investigate the history of these forms in Nahuatl on their own, it is Brian Stubbs. And really, it don't seem that he has even tried to look into the immediate history of any of the Nahuatl terms he cites. He has just assumed that it was conservative. Investigating alternative explanations of one's data is of course a basic part of establishing a hypothesis in a rigorous manner.
I should note, that since many of the reconstructions of proto-Corachol and proto-Corachol Nahuatl are my own and most of them yet unpublished, Stubbs cannot of course be faulted for not knowing or accepting them. But if anyone in the world would be equipped with enough knowledge to investigate the history of these forms in Nahuatl on their own, it is Brian Stubbs. And really, it don't seem that he has even tried to look into the immediate history of any of the Nahuatl terms he cites. He has just assumed that it was conservative. Investigating alternative explanations of one's data is of course a basic part of establishing a hypothesis in a rigorous manner.
I am not equipped to
evaluate Stubbs’ usage of the data from many of the other UA languages, nor the
Semitic or Egyptian, but given how the Nahuatl is treated, it cannot simply be assumed
that it is being handled well. I pass the baton to the next scholars to check how he handles the languages of their expertise, both Uto-Aztecan and Afro-Asiatic.
J. S. Gould also showed us that we all as scholars are prone
to the error of confirmation bias. Dearly held beliefs, whether religious,
political or theoretical, are likely to color our interpretation of data and
dull our critical sense about our own conclusions. I think as a discipline, historical
linguistics, because it relies on our ability to see intricate patterns that
others have not yet seen, is more prone to being influenced by our biases in interpretations than
most other kinds of science. Really, I think historical linguistics is perhaps
more of an art, though an art that should be approached with a scientific
mindset. This proneness to confirmation bias of course no less affects me as an non-theist scholar with a distinct set of ideas about what happened in prehistoric Mesoamerica, than it does people of other diverse persuasions and ideas. But this is why these kinds of scholarly endeavors have to stand and fall on the evaluation of empirical data by people with different biases: we are all entitled to our interpretations, but no one is entitled to their own facts.
What would be needed for a proposal like this to be convincing to me? First of all
the question is, how much will be left once experts in different other languages involved weed out the infelicitous examples as I have done here. This sample suggests that quite little would be left after such a pass-through. But the next version of this proposal should also take some steps to remedy the basic methodological flaws:
- I would very much prefer non-linguistic (that is archaeological or genetic) corroboration of ancient trans-atlantic contact before I would entertain the hypothesis of contact between Uto-Aztecan and ancient Afroasiatic languages as a reasonable explanation of likenesses between the languages.
- I would want a much more thorough description of the proposed borrowings, including vowels. Uto-Aztecans would have heard the semitic words with vowels, and borrowed them with vowels. So it is simply not possible to simply ignore the vowels as Stubbs does (exept when he finds one that accidentally fits). I would expect systematic vowel patterns for verbs, nouns etc. Also forms with partial matches, where only two of three consonants match, cannot be allowed.
- I would want attention paid to chronology. When did the supposed borrowings take place? Already before proto-Uto-Aztecan? In that case all proposed borrowings must be reconstructible to PUA and to which ever layer of Afro-Asiatic or Semitic one believes was spoken at that period. It is not reasonable to cherry pick forms from the individual daughter languages and claim that they are retentions when the may as well be innovations (such retentions can only be posited after the relationship is established).
- As Chris Rogers’ points out a convincing proposal would in fact have much fewer correspondence pairs, of much higher quality, and preferably, they would be paradigmatically related. For example, what is closest to appear convincing for example is the pronominal forms, where the n, t- y- pattern looks highly intriguing (until you remember that the first person n- is pan-American and realize the y- doesn’t fit).
One thing that makes me uncomfortable is the fact that Stubbs worked on this simultaneously with working on his catalogue of Uto-Aztecan reconstructions. I cannot help but worry that the reconstructions of UA forms there, may be subtly compromised by Stubbs unconsciously trying to make them fit with his Semitic data. I will have to use it with a degree of apprehension in the future.
Texts Cited/Mentioned:
- Elzinga, Dirk. 2016. "Review of Exploring the Explanatory Power of Semitic and Egyptian in Uto-Aztecan" in BYU Studies Quarterly. 55(4):172-176
- Rogers, Chris. 2019. “A Review of the Afro-Asiatic: Uto-Aztecan Proposal” Journal of Book of Mormon Studies, Vol. 28, 258-267
- Robertson, John S. 2017. "Exploring Semitic and Egyptian in Uto-Aztecan Languages".
- Interpreter: A Journal of Latter-day Saint Faith and Scholarship 25: 103-116
- Stubbs, Brian D. 2015. Exploring the Explanatory Power of Semitic and Egyptian in Uto-Aztecan. Grover Publications.
- Stubbs, Brian D. 2011. Uto-aztecan: a comparative vocabulary. By Brian D. Stubbs. Blanding, Utah: Rocky Mountain Books and Publications
full disclosure: My name is Beau Anderson. I am LDS and am engaged in LDS apologetics.
SvarSletThank you for taking time to do this review. As I am not a trained historical linguist I won't comment too much on the specifics of the examples you evaluate here, but having spent substantial time trying to understand the strength of Stubbs' proposal, there are a few things in your review that concern me:
1. The review states that "most of [Stubbs'] comparisons are to his own reconstructed proto-Uto-Aztecan forms"
While it is true that most of Brians comparisons contain numbered references so that they can be looked up in his Proto-Uto-Aztecan dictionary, this statement masks the fact that in both the PUA dictionary and in the proposal in question Brian does an excellent job of prominently presenting relevant reconstructions from other authors whenever applicable, even when they differ from his own reconstructions. In any case, the PUA dictionary itself is very well respected among Uto-Aztecanists.
2. "he does on many occasions reach into Nahuan and compare Nahuan forms directly with forms in individual semitic varieties. Looking closely at these examples will give us feel for how Stubbs handles linguistic data"
If I understand this second quote from your review correctly, it tells us why you are choosing to limit the review to a few particular examples instead of focusing on the vast majority of the data in which the Nahuan forms support PUA reconstructions.
What is confusing to me is that after you first tell us that you are excluding the Nahuan forms that do, in fact, reconstruct to PUA forms, one of your main complaints about many of the individual forms is that they don't reconstruct to PUA forms. That seems like circular logic. The vast majority of Nahuatl forms in Stubbs' proposal ARE presented in support of reconstructions, but you focus on the others as if they can help the reader understand the proposal, then complain about the very criteria by which you chose those examples.
3. In criticizing the cognate for the Egyptian crocodile-god Sobek, you exclude an important point, which is that Brian Stubbs is not the first historical linguist to promote the idea that this example might be evidence of language connection between Egyptian and Nahuatl. I believe Cyrus Gordon was the first to propose this connection (see Cyrus Gordon, 1971 p135).
4. You assert that Stubbs' gains semantic leeway by virtue of Coptic examples, but this is not true. He specifically states that the Coptic forms that he includes are included because they give an indication of the voweling which is not orthographic in Late Egyptian. I do not remember seeing a single Coptic form that was used as a comparison. They were always presented beside forms from prior stages of Egyptian. Something similar may apply to the rare Syriac forms that he mentions, although I'd have to look to refresh my memory. I believe that leading the reader to believe that Stubbs' proposal doesn't have specific focus on NW-Semitic and Late Egyptian forms does a disservice. Perhaps you can find some examples among his 1500 that you don't agree with in relation to NW-Semitic or Late Egyptian, I don't know, but the vast majority of his proposal does not pull from outside of those boundaries. When other forms are mentioned they are largely or entirely in support of forms within those boundaries.
1. This is irrelevant. It is not a problem that he uses his own reconstructions, this is fine. Unless, that is, his own reconstructions have been unconsicously influenced by the idea of a corresponding semitic etymon. We can't know if this is the case, but we can look at how he treats the data he compares from specific languages. In this case this is not reassuring because it seems he is at times stretching the data beyond what it can bear. I mention that the reconstructions are his own, because reconstructions are a creative process that are inevitably influenced by the reconstructors ideas about the past of the language. Ideally a comparison like this would not involved reconstructions made by the person seeking to prove the relation (though I realize that in practice they many proposals do use the authors own reconstructions - but this is problematic (for example the same problem exists with the recent toto-zoquean proposal, where reconstructions by other scholars fail to support the proposal).
Slet2. Yes, I am not reviwing the entire proposal. I am using the instances where we can see how he uses data from a specific language as a sample, that can give a feeling for his overall comparative rigor. It may be the case that the other correspondences comparing with PUA reconstructions are better, but a reconstruction is never better than the rigor with which one treats the specific data. That is why it is relevant to look at this in the only examples where I am qualified to critique his use of data from a specific language (namely for Nahuatl). Sure I could extend the critique to the way he uses Nahuatl in his reconstructions, but that would require something much bigger than a blog post.
3. yes this is true, but I don't see how it is relevant that some other scholar (with no expertise in any Native American language, but even if they had), has made the same observation of the similarity earlier.
4. You may be right that his use of Coptic does not add semantic leeway, because of the way he uses it. But this is largely irrelevant for my critique since the leeway gained by the other languages used and by the lax criteria for semantic matches and by the lack of attention to histories of innovation in the individual languages, provides a semantic leeway so wide that it more than accounts for the likenesses as probable examples of simple chance similarity.
As your review also leans on the review by Rogers, I would point readers to Stubbs' response to Rogers' review: https://mormanity.blogspot.com/2019/08/guest-post-from-brian-stubbs-response.html
SvarSletIt is already linked and summarized.
SletI found your review of 'bow' > 'rainbow' quite convincing until I looked at what Stubbs' actually presented.
SvarSlet1. You say that you avoided reviewing examples that reconstruct, then you completely miss seeing that almost the entirety of the presentation of the data for this form relates to its PUA reconstruction (unless all you did was look at the one-line summary on page 7 instead of his actual presentation of the form on page 118).
2. The following statement is factually incorrect: "he neglects to remove the final –t which *is* a suffix and *should* be removed."
Stubbs *does* remove the -t suffix like you say he *should* and is very clear about it in his presentation of the data (unless all you did was look at the one-line summary on page 7 instead of his actual presentation of the form on page 118).
3. You say that Stubbs dismisses koo- as a prefix, which is incorrect. He doesn't dismiss it, he identifies it as PUA koNwa 'snake', which Stubbs says "is often in UA words for colorful things
like rainbows, because of many snakes’ bright and varied colors; thus, the koo- of CN koo-samaloo-tl" (again, this is clear in the actual presentation of Stubbs' data on page 118, but not on the one-liner that your review might have been based on).
If we ignore Stubbs' presentation of the form and consider that the koo- is actually ko- < kosa- from kos-tik 'yellow' like you suggest, then it would be helpful to know where the sibilant went, because it is present in the related forms that you present:
kosawi 'become yellow'
koskatl 'necklace'
4. You say "[Stubbs] should compare šmrt with ksml, but that doesn’t look very much a like at all." This is again a factual mistake. Stubbs *does* compare šmrt with PUA ko(C)-samalo. Again, please read the actual presentation of his data on page 118 because this is very obvious.
5. You state the following "the Nahuatl word does not appear related to the shape at all, but rather to the color, and Nahua myth compares it to a snake not a bow". Here you might have accidentally stumbled closer to Stubbs' presentation. You found your way to 'snake' (see Stubbs' relation of snake > color above and in the data on page 118).
You seem not to really follow what I am arguing here, perhaps I was unclear.
Slet1. Kosamalotl does not reconstruct for PUA, maximally for SUA, but I don't buy Stubbs attempt to do so. The kihona/kenola/vainora/orawi forms look to me as a completely different etymon that can only reconstruct for SUA and which to me appears unrelated to the attested Nahuan form. (neither vowels nor consonants match well)
2. Yes, Stubbs removes the final t from the Nahuatl form but it is present in the semitic form he compares with (*smrt). So the "match" is only for three consonants out of four (which is not good). But not even that because samalo is *not the root*, the roots in the Nahua word are kosa + malo.
3. That is not what I say, I say he decides it is a prefix in nuahuan and can therefore be removed from the comparison, so that he compares samalo(t) with smrt. Yes, Stubbs argues that ko is a prefix meaning snake. There is however no such prefix in Nahuan, and hence also not in the word kosamalotl, which as I am arguing is a compound noun made from kosa+malo. So the sibiliant didn't go anywhere, it is right there, in the word. Additional support for the fact that the root is *kosa comes from Cora and Huichol where rainbow is kuusa, with no *malo.
5. Yes, he makes that claim in order to try to make the ko related to snake, but I don't think it is - if it were it would have to be *kosa that meant snake, not ko- (and then the samalo/smrt comparison would still not work). That Nahuans see the rainbow as a snake is irrelevant if we do not consider ko- in kosamalotl related to the word for snake *koowa.
1. You say the kihona/kenola/vainora/orawi forms appear unrelated to CN koosamaaloo", but again you are omitting relevant information presented by Stubbs by ignoring NT kiihónali and TO gihonalï, and his discussion of these and other forms.
SletAre you arguing against Tepiman h < *s? Or against Tepiman often m > n? These give kiisómali and gisonalï, which compare better to CN koosamaaloo than the forms you chose in your response might otherwise lead readers to believe.
2. Correct about final -t.
3. You are correct that I misjudged the sibilant from 'yellow', but since you yourself described the association of the Nahuatl form with 'snake' and 'color' semantics without realizing that Stubbs was also arguing for the involvement of those semantics, I find myself skeptical that your 'yellow' is a better argument than Stubbs' 'snake/colorful' argument.
4. Assuming you agree with #4.
5. Now you're saying "That Nahuans see the rainbow as a snake is irrelevant"? That's different than what you said in the review. You independently made the association with snake/color in the review.
I am not "omitting" information, that is a silly accusation, for not including all the forms proposed by Stubbs. Kiihónali also is a poor match to kosamalotl. I cannot say how frequent the s/h and n/m correspondence is in Tepiman (it is not a regular correspondence to be sure), but even if there was a kiihónali form that is only close to kosamalo if you ignore the vowels. Stubbs simply doesn't do the work required to link the SUA forms together, and much less to link the Nahuan form with the other SUA forms, and there are better explanations for the Nahua form by looking at the elements internally in Nahuatl and in Corachol. The kihonali form doesnt even fit with the snake proposal since snake is no kii/gii in tepiman, but ko'o. So basically, you can't have both kiihonali be a cognate of kosamalotl and ko- being a "snake" prefix, it is either or. If there is a SUA root (still only SUA, not PUA) root for rainbow that is *kosamalo with the reflex kiihónali in Tepiman then he should compare ksmr with smrt, which is not a good match at all and leaves both the k and the t unaccounted for.
Slet3. Obviously I realize that Stubbs was proposing that snake and rainbow was related, that is of course why I included the fact that they relate the rainbow to snakes for the sake of being fair (though that is not clear). But again it is irrelevant since there is no "snake" element in the Nahua word.
4. obviously I don't- and no he doesn't. Stubbs compares smrt with sml, having (wrongly) argued that ko is a prefix that can be discarded from the Nahua form.
5. Actually that was too strong a claim on my part, Nahuas mythologically relate the rainbow to a snake, there is no reason to think they see it as one. And yes, it is irrelevant because there is no element meaning "snake" in the word kosamalotl.
You say "there is no element meaning 'snake' in the word kosamalotl", but you also say kosamalotl often means 'snake' and I haven't noticed you disagree with Stubbs' argument that 'snake' "is often in UA words for colorful things like rainbows, because of many snakes’ bright and varied colors".
SletWhat is missing doesn't seem to be a segment for 'snake', what is missing seems to be the semantics 'yellow'.
No, I am not saying that kosamalotl ever means snake, because it does not. Please don't twist my words. I am saying that Nahuas see the rainbow as related to a snake-deity. This is not the same thing as saying there is a semantic relation between ko- in kosamalotl and snakes.
SletThat's fair, I didn't mean to overstep what you said. You only referred to snake in relation to a mythological figure / deity, but calling it irrelevant just because it strengthens his point is nonsense.
SletFirst of all, you didn't even need to look beyond Stubbs' book to see that your kooaatl 'snake deity' semantics are cognate with 'snake' in PUA:
"Mn toqoqqwa 'snake'; Mn patagówa 'watersnake'; Mn toġóqa 'rattlesnake'; NP togoggwa ‘rattlesnake’; TSh koko 'gopher snake'; TSh pa-suku/tokowa ‘water snake’;
Sh tokoa ‘snake, rattlesnake’; Sh kokon ‘bull snake, blow snake’; Sh pasinkokon ‘water snake’; Kw tokowa 'rattlesnake'; Kw koko 'gopher snake'; SP toŋoa-vi 'rattlesnake'; CU togoa-vi; TO ko’oi/ko’owi 'rattlesnake'; Nv ko’o; PYp ko’o; NT kói/kóyi; ST ko’; Eu vakoc 'culebra'; Yq báakot; My baákot; Wr kuhuá 'snake sp.'; Tbr koó-t; Wc kúú; Cr ku’uku’u-se 'snakes'; Cr kuku (Sapir); CN kooaa-tl 'snake, serpent, worm, twin';
Pl kuuwa-t 'snake’. Munro (1973) includes Ls qiqeŋ-la 'ring snake' (with reduplication), Cp qeqeŋi-ly ‘king snake’ (Ls loan?) and shows *w as one source for Ls ŋ and so for other Tak languages as well..."
In fact, in your review of 'Serpent/Twin' you quote from ^that^ exact paragraph, so it's obvious that you know that there are many UA 'snake' cognates for your 'snake deity'.
Your only argument for it being irrelevant relies on your 'yellow' argument, but 'yellow' is not in the semantics of kosamalotl while semantics like 'colorful' and 'snake deity' are.
Correction to that last sentence: it should not say 'snake deity', just 'snake'
SletI don't know what you are even talking about anymore. Of course koowatl is cognate with PUA *ku. But those words not cognate of kosamalotl but of koowatl. That is the *only* word in Nahuatl that I know of that has a reflex of PUA *ko "snake".
SletI am not claiming that the meaning of *kosa is necessarily yellow, what I am claiming is that the root is kosa and not -ko. And that we can see this because the root kosa can be reconstructed based on the words, kostik, kosawi, kosahtli "weasel", koskatl "necklace", and that it may have included a meaning of yellow, but potentially also other meanings like something elongated (necklace, weasel) - or even colorful. The same root is evidence by Corachol kuusa "rainbow".
In regards to your review that you present as "wine skin" > "prickly pear":
SvarSlet1. You quote "Syriac nbl3/3n’bl", but that is not an accurate quote from Stubbs' presentation. To clarify, Stubbs presents "Syriac nbl/n’bl".
2. You describe this as "a very bad semantic match" and that "allowing this span from wineskin to cactus with a fruit used occasionally for fermented shows an very high degree of semantic latitude".
This certainly sounds like a tremendous amount of semantic latitude, but is because your review only presents a subset of the Semitic information and a subset of the PUA information that Stubbs provides. To fill in the semantic blank, here is more of what Stubbs provides about the source semantics:
"the meaning of the root nbl is uncertain, yet another identical root nbl means ‘be senseless, foolish’ [as when drunk]".
Stubbs then compares it to PUA napai ‘acoholic drink, drunk’
The "prickly pear" examples that follow would indeed seem like a huge semantic stretch if Stubbs had not first demonstrated the relatedness to PUA napai. In light of napai, PUA napol/napoi ‘prickly pear cactus/fruit’ (which Stubbs shows as existing in at least 20 languages of the Num, Tak, Hp, Tep, TrC, CrC, Azt branches) still contains semantic leeway, but far less than your review recognizes.
I would recognize that since the napol/napoi examples rely on the napai examples to reduce semantic leeway, an argument can be made that the napol/napoi forms do not meet Campbell's requirement that comparative forms be able to stand alone.
You are right that Stubbs in his comparative vocabulary relates the root napa to a root na'pul/no'pal "prickly pear". In fact, he puts the root for prickly pear under the entry "alcohol", which he does not justify much less "demonstrate" (it is more like a claim). Now I worry that this is indeed a sign that he has been keeping his potential semitic cognates in mind when he organized the vocabulary, otherwise I really don't see why the two roots *napa "drunk/alcohol" and *na'pul "prickly pear" should be considered related. It would have made more sense to include *na'pul under the entry cactus. Also the reconstruction is odd, he reconstructs an l, even though only Nahuan has final l and no other language in his sample has a corresponding segment (also no other language has a reflex of the h). Why does he do that? (to make the reconstruction a better match for *nbl? That would be worrying)
SletSo yes, you are right that if we accept his proposal of a deep connection in UA between the prickly pear and a word for alcohol/drunkenness, then the semantic match is slightly better. But I don't see a convincing argument for why we should. So yes, my argument about excessive semantic leeway stands.
Stubbs' argument that the final -l corresponds to Tepiman -i might add up when we consider the dipthong in NT návoi, but I am not qualified to say one way or the other. There is regularity to this in other forms. I came across a good description of it while looking at your 'tamale' review:
Slet"I include the liquid l in the reconstruction due to (1) its presence in CN, (2) the general lack of proto-dipthongs in UA, which dipthongs are usually due to loss of an intervening C or assimilation (i.e., ai < *aCi or aiCi < *aCi), (3) the fact that UA liquids often encourage assimilation toward, if not become, high front vowels (*l > i/ï), and (4) the presence of such a high front vowel in other reflexes where CN's liquid is"
Ok, I had missed that proposed explanation. I am not convinced.
SletI think it is probably easier for those looking at your review to be able to actually reference Stubb's book at http://bmslr.org/books/Exploring%20the%20Explanatory%20Power%20of%20Semitic%20and%20Egyptian%20in%20Uto-Aztecan.pdf . One question, some of your assessment involves the chronology of the languages, what do you consider the dates for proto-Nahuatl, Classical Nahuatl / Nahuatl? And what would be the source material for those dates (I'm assuming that most have agreed that glottochronology is not considered a useful method)?
SvarSletYes, dating and chronology is important, and another major weakness of Stubbs proposal since he mixes varieties from such a wide span of dates in his comparison.
SletOpinions vary on the different forms of glottochronology, and they definitely are not absolute dating methods, but they do allow a certain degree of ability to compare relative splitting dates of different language groups.
What we call classical Nahuatl is postcolonial, but the emergence of something that is similar to classical Nahuatl dates to probably something like 8-900 CE at the earliest.
Proto-Nahuatl would split up into Eastern and Western branches around 6-700 CE at the latest.
And in my view proto-Nahuatl would itself split gradually from proto-Corachol-Nahua in the period between 1 CE and 500 CE.
So this is all much later than the Biblical chronology.
Magnus, as I understand it, Stubbs has not argued exactly when semitic and egyptian influenced UA. It could have been (or not?) that an existing spoken language or even literary or scholarly language based on semitic and egyptian influences was preserved and eventually influenced UA at a later date. Kind of like how Latin has influenced English even without taking Norman French into account. I mean, how could Latin, a dead language, determine English's use of terms like "per se" or "etc?"
SvarSletAnd what about Norman French? Could semitic or egyptian have grown into a language that influenced UA as Latin grew into French and influenced English? I think that there are some assumptions you are making that could be addressed if you spoke with Stubbs. Have you talked with him about your concerns? I'm sure he would be happy to talk or correspond with you.
Denne kommentar er fjernet af forfatteren.
SletYes, we have in fact already had a friendly exchange by email about my points of critique, some of which he seems to acknowledge and others of which he considers to be mistaken.
SletArguing when is crucial. You have to have a when, because UA is one "when" and Southern UA or proto-Aztecan are completely different "whens" each separated by thousands of years.
If we are now postulating a continuous presence of Egyptian and/or Hebrew/Semitic speakers in the Americas for several thousand years, then it becomes really strange that there is zero written, archeological, or DNA evidence of their presence, and no similar patterns of language contact with non-UA languages, not to mention the question of where they all went.
Do you know that there's zero written, archeological or DNA evidence of their presence?
SletI don't think that Stubbs argues for the Book of Mormon in his books (I haven't read them, but I've read articles by him, in favor of him and seen a speech he gave). But if one were defending the Book of Mormon, using Stubb's research, then it would be helpful to understand what the Book of Mormon actually claims. If you knew that, you wouldn't have said that the contact was necessarily thousands of years ago. The Book of Mormon does not demand that. You also don't seem to realize that an estimated 90% of native mexican peoples were killed by european diseases, not to mention the Book of Mormon's claims that a genocidal war destroyed most of the relevant people. One of your criticisms is that he seeks for a number of cognates which is merely a matter of chance given the total number of languages in UA and Semitic. But what is powerful to me about Stubbs' work is the patterns he identifies that have explanatory power. I don't think you adequately address this especially when you claim his work is not falsifiable. When Stubbs identifies a pattern of changing one Semitic language (Aramaic) from b to p and then another Semitic language (possibly phoenician) from b to kw-, he is setting out a pattern that could be falsified. Furthermore, the “p” dialect is the one with the Egyptian in it, but not the kw- dialect. If the "p" language had examples of semitic cognates that went from b to d then that would weaken or falsify his theory. But if not, then his theory has explanatory power that sheds helpful light on Uto-Aztecan and the origin of some of its vocabulary. But I’m not a linguist so I apologize if this is totally off-base.
Yes, there is zero such evidence that has been accepted by non-mormon scholars.
SletThe book of Mormon does not demand contact a thousand years ago perhaps, but UA does. If you want to postulate contact with proto-Uto-Aztecan then that would necessarily have been at least 4-5000 ago. That is when there was a single Uto-Aztecan language, any contact after that would have been with separate Uto-Aztecan languages, and most of the postulated cognates are reconstructed for PUA.
The patterns do not have any explanatory power if they are based on data that is not valid or if they are based on data that is simply chance resemblances. The idea of explanatory power by the way is a very odd idea in this case, since he is claiming contact and not shared inheritance. You don't expect regular sound laws in borrowed vocabulary. And the phenomena that he claims need explanation in UA, can in fact be explained in a number of other and much more plausible ways, as other UA scholars have already done.
The claim that 90% were destroyed by European diseases is of course true, but irrelevant since that happened after European contact, and it would have been registered if hebrew speakers had inhabited the US Southwest untill the 1500s. And since both egyptians and hebrews were literate people they would have left writings and inscriptions in their languages if they were there for a thousand years, and they did not.
Regarding your first comment, can you tell me what literature review you did to conclude that there is "zero" evidence? An extraordinary claim. Or are you so certain that a non-Mormon would never break ranks in that way? This sounds like institutional bias, unless you can point me to some comprehensive literature reviews.
SletThe 90% comment was in response to your question as to why we don't find the DNA and evidence of the people who may have spoken semitic languages. Our archaeology of mesoamerica may very well be in its infancy, with major discoveries still happening. https://www.nationalgeographic.com/news/2018/02/maya-laser-lidar-guatemala-pacunam/
When you say, "You don't expect regular sound laws in borrowed vocabulary" Really? Can you point me to some sources because I find this very counter intuitive.
Would we need to go back to proto-germanic to find influence of Latin in English? Can't we find french influence in English from 1066 CE onward? And can't we expect that influence to be different for Scots English (very little) and Cajun English (quite a lot)?
Let me ask you one last thing. What if every word in a particular UA language was similar to every corresponding word/idea in Aramaic? Surely then you would agree that there must be a relationship. Or would you, even then, say that there needs to be evidence of archaeological contact?
thanks.
Oh, I don't know, just the kind of literature review that comes from 20+ years of dedicated professional level study specializing in Mesoamerican language culture and history. Somehow I think someone would have told me, or I would have read about it sometime during that period if there was any accepted evidence of precolumbian transatlantic contact, or even if there was mildly promising evidence. There is not. And yes, we will still learn more, but so far no evidence *whatsoever* suggests such contact.
SletIt is very basic stuff in historical linguistics since the 19th century that regular sound correspondences are evidence of a genetic relation while borrowing can be identified primarily because it does not follow the regular patterns. You can open pretty much any textbook on historical linguistics or borrowing to find this.
Germanic languages have been influenced by Latin for the past 2000 years *and still are*. Stubbs claims that semitic influence can be reconstructed for PUA and that the contact was with speakers of PUA.
What do you mean by similar? How similar is similar? The problem is that what Stubbs considers to be similar really is not very similar, at least not more similar than would be expected from pure chance.
Now, you must have me excused, but I don't think I shall respond to more of your comments, they are getting a little high strung here.
Very well. I did not mean to be insulting. Best wishes.
SletThat last comment was from me, Collin Simonsen. I don't know why it calls it "unknown" now.
SvarSletIf I am correct, I think there is not really consensus on the date of split of Uto-Aztecan, I think J. Hill has proposed the separation of proto-Uto-Aztecan at 2500 BP, which I think would probably be consistent with the Stubbs analysis. Also there are some traces of Semitic/Egyptian that have been identified by non-LDS scholars. S.C. Compton (2010) has identified a direct correspondence between the Paleo-Hebrew alphabet and the Maya day names as well as between the Egyptian phonetic glyph sounds and the Mayan syllabic glyph sounds. David Kelley (1960)identified a correspondence between the some Maya day names and Hebrew letters, with those letters being the exclusive New Year day days throughout all of Mesoamerica (excepting the Olmec) (Edmonson, 1988). So there are traces of some influence.
SvarSletIs this project still on-going? Just found this blog - very interesting!
SvarSletDenne kommentar er fjernet af en blogadministrator.
SvarSlet